May 25, 2016
It may make us uncomfortable, but animal research saves human and animal lives. Understanding Animal Research/Wellcome |
The
media regularly report impressive medical advances. However, in most cases,
there is a reluctance by scientists, the universities, or research institutions
they work for, and the media to mention animals used in that research, let
alone non-human primates. Such omission misleads the public and works against
long-term sustainability of a very important means of advancing knowledge about
health and disease.
Consider
the recent report by Ali Rezai and colleagues, in
the journal Nature, of a patient with quadriplegia who was able to use his
hands by just thinking about the action. The signals in the brain recorded by
implanted electrodes were analysed and fed into the muscles of the arm to
activate the hand directly.
When
journalists report on such bionic devices, rarely is there mention of the
decades of research using macaques that eventually made these early
brain-machine interfaces a reality for human patients. The public is shielded
from this fact, thereby lending false credence to claims by animal rights
groups that medical breakthroughs come from human trials with animal
experiments playing no part.
Development
of such brain-machine interfaces requires detailed understanding of how the
primate brain processes information and many experiments on macaques using
different interfaces and computing algorithms. Human ethics committees will not
let you try this on a patient until such animal research is done.
These
devices are still not perfect and our understanding of brain function at a
neuronal level needs more sophistication. In some cases, the macaque neural
circuitry one discovers may not quite match the human’s, but usually it is as
close as we can get to the human scenario, needing further fine-tuning in
direct human trials. However, to eliminate all animal research and try
everything out on humans without much inkling of their effects is dangerous and
therefore highly unethical.
The
technique Dr Rezai’s team used on human patients draws heavily upon work
done on monkeys by many groups. This can be seen by looking at
the paper and the references it cites.
Another
case in point is the technique of deep
brain stimulation using implanted electrodes, which is becoming an effective
means of treating symptoms in many Parkinson’s patients. This is now
possible largely due to the decades of work on macaques to understand in
detail the complex circuitry involved in motor control. Macaques continue
to be used to refine deep brain stimulation in humans.
Ethical choices
The
number of monkeys used for such long-term neuroscience experiments is
relatively small, with just two used in the study above. Many more
are used for understanding disease processes and developing treatment methods
or vaccines in the case of infectious diseases such as malaria, Ebola,
HIV/AIDS, tuberculosis and Zika.
Approximately
60,000 monkeys are used for experiments for all purposes each year in the United States, Europe and Australia.
However,
if one looks at what is at stake without these experiments on non-human
primates, one must acknowledge a stark reality. In many cases, the situation is
similar to that which once existed with polio. Nearly 100,000 monkeys were used
in the 1950s to develop the polio vaccine. Before that, millions of people
worldwide, mostly children, were infected with polio every year. Around 10% died and many were left crippled.
Now,
thanks to the vaccine, polio is almost eradicated.
Similarly,
about 200 million people contract malaria every year, of
whom 600,000 (75% being children) die, despite all efforts to control
the mosquitoes that transmit the disease. Development of a vaccine is our best
chance, but again primates are necessary for this, as other
species are not similarly susceptible to the parasitic infection.
Circumstances
are similar with other devastating ailments such as Ebola, HIV and Zika. The
ethical choice is often between using a few hundred monkeys or condemning
thousands or more humans to suffer or die from each one of these diseases year
after year.
In
the popular press and in protests against primate research, there is sometimes
no distinction made between great apes (chimpanzees, bonobos and gorillas) and
monkeys such as macaques, leading to misplaced emotional reactions. To my
knowledge, invasive experiments on great apes are not done anywhere, because of
the recognition of their cognitive proximity to humans.
While
the ape and human lineages separated six million years ago, there is an additional 20 to 35 million years of
evolutionary distance from monkeys, which clearly lack the sophisticated
cognitive capacities of the apes.
With
urgent medical issues of today such as HIV, Ebola, malaria, Zika, diabetes and
neurological conditions such as stroke and Parkinson’s disease, monkeys are
adequate to study the basic physiology and pathology and to develop treatment
methods. There is nothing extra to be gained from studying apes.
Alternatives have limitations
Opponents
of animal research often cite the impressive developments of computer
modelling, in-vitro techniques and non-invasive experiments in humans as
alternatives to animal experiments. These have indeed given us great insights
and are frequently used also by the very same scientists who use animals.
However,
there are still critical areas where animal experimentation will be required
for a long time to come.
Modelling
can be done only on data already obtained and therefore can only build upon the
hypotheses such data supported. The modelling also needs validation by going
back to the lab to know whether the model’s predictions are correct.
Real
science cannot work in a virtual world. It is the synergy between computation
and real experiments that advances computational research.
In-vitro
studies on isolated cells from a cell line cultured in the lab or directly
taken from an animal are useful alternatives. This approach is widely used in
medical research. However, these cells are not the same as the complex system
provided by the whole animal. Unless one delves into the physiology and
pathology of various body functions and tries to understand how they relate to
each other and to the environment, any insights gained from studying single
cells in in-vitro systems will be limited.
Though
many studies can be done non-invasively on humans and we have indeed gained
much knowledge on various questions, invasive experiments on animals are
necessary. In many human experiments we can study the input to the system and
the output, but we are fairly limited in understanding what goes on in between.
For example, interactions between diet,
the microbiome, the digestive system and disease are so complex that
important relationships that have to be understood to advance therapy can only
be worked out in animal models.
Of
course, animals are not perfect models for the human body. They can never be.
Species evolve and change.
However,
many parts of our bodies have remained the same over millions of years of
evolution. In fact, much of our basic knowledge about how impulses are
transmitted along a nerve fibre has come from studying
the squid, but our understanding also gets gradually modified by
more recent experiments in mammals.
Higher
cognitive functions and the complex operations of the motor system have to be
studied in mammals. For a small number of these studies, nothing less than a
non-human primate is adequate.
The
choice of species for every experiment is usually carefully considered by
investigators, funding bodies and ethics committees, from both ethical and
scientific viewpoints. That is why the use of non-human primates is usually a
small percentage of all animals used for research. In the state of Victoria,
this constitutes only 0.02%.
Medical
history can vouch for the fact that the benefits from undertaking animal
experiments are worth the effort in the long run and that such experimentation
is sometimes the only ethical choice. Taken overall, the principle of least
harm should and does prevail. There may come a day when non-invasive
experiments in humans may be able to tell us almost everything that animal
experiments do today, but that is probably still a long way off.
Priorities in animal use
The
ethical pressure put on research seems to be in stark contrast to that on the
food industry. It is hypocritical for a society to contemplate seriously
restricting the use of the relatively small number of animals for research that
could save lives when far more animals are allowed to be slaughtered just to
satisfy the palate. This is despite meat being a health and environmental
concern.
To
put this in perspective, for every animal used in research (mostly mice, fish
and rats), approximately 2,000 animals are used for food, with actual numbers varying between countries and the organisations that collect the data.
The
ratio becomes even more dramatic when you consider the use of non-human
primates alone. In Victoria, for every monkey used in research, more than one million animals are used for meat production.
However, the monitoring of the welfare of farm animals is not in any way
comparable to that which experimental animals receive.
Reduced
use of livestock can greatly reduce mankind’s ecological footprint
and also improve our health. This is an ethical, health and environmental
imperative. Animal experiments, including some on non-human primates, are also
an ethical and medical imperative.
https://theconversation.com/we-mightnt-like-it-but-there-are-ethical-reasons-to-use-animals-in-medical-research-58878
No comments:
Post a Comment