Pages

Monday, July 4, 2016

What Doctors Know About How Bad It Is, and Won’t Say

CreditDavid Plunkert

Even terminally ill patients still receive scant information, researchers have found, while family members acting for I.C.U. patients commonly contend with confusion and misinformation. The studies also uncover some reasons for the disconnect. Doctors, it seems, shouldn’t get all the blame.
Dr. Holly Prigerson, director of the Center for Research on End-of-Life Careat Weill Cornell Medicine, and her colleagues interviewed 178 patients at cancer centers across the country. All had cancers that had progressed despite chemotherapy; their oncologists estimated their life expectancy at less than six months. “These patients were all dying, and everyone treating them was well aware of it,” Dr. Prigerson said.
Yet nearly 40 percent said they’d never discussed prognosis or life expectancy with their oncologists. Not surprisingly, when asked to answer four key questions about how well they understood their illness — including whether they grasped that their cancer was incurable and that they had months, not years, to live — only 5 percent answered correctly.
Researchers once attributed such results to communication failures: Doctors weren’t talking about prognosis or they weren’t using language that lay people understood.
“We made a lot of assumptions that with good information people would make good, clear, rational decisions,” said Dr. Douglas White, who directs a program on ethical decision making at the University of Pittsburgh.
Reality turns out to be considerably more complicated.
In Dr. White’s earlier research, asking surrogates with relatives in intensive care to respond to hypothetical prognoses, family members were markedly more likely to correctly interpret optimistic predictions (“a 90 percent chance of surviving”) than pessimistic ones (“a 5 percent chance”).
That’s called “optimism bias.” But the team’s latest study, in JAMA, shows that it is hardly the only muddying factor.
Dr. White and his colleagues surveyed 229 surrogates and 99 physicians involved in the care of critically ill patients, most older than65, who’d already spent five days on ventilators in intensive care. The researchers asked physicians to estimate how likely each patient was to survive the hospitalization, from 0 percent to 100 percent; they asked family members what they thought their physician’s prognosis was.
More than half the time, the researchers found substantial “discordance” — at least a 20-percentage-point difference between the physician’s assessment and what the surrogates thought it would be.
Nearly half the time, a misunderstanding explained why the surrogates were off base. The study wasn’t really designed to tell whose fault that was. But often the beliefs of family members — a brew of personal, emotional and spiritual convictions at a fraught time — also played a role. In interviews, such surrogates talked about the importance of “good vibrations, the power of positive thinking to actually change the outcome,” Dr. White said. Believing in recovery, they said, might help bring it about.
Or they reflected “the Lake Wobegon effect,” named for Garrison Keillor’s fictional town where all the children are above average: Many patients in this situation might die, but their relatives were “fighters” or had other unique strengths, so the usual odds didn’t apply. Alternatively, the family’s religious beliefs sometimes dictated that whatever the doctors thought, only God could determine the patient’s future.
Understanding what lies ahead can profoundly affect patients’ quality of life — and death. If they underestimate their life expectancy, they may forgo helpful treatment. If they overestimate it — the more common misperception — they may agree to tests and procedures that turn their final weeks and months into a medical treadmill.
Frank discussions don’t disrupt the bond between doctors and patients, Dr. Prigerson has shown. They do increase the likelihood that patients receive the end-of-life care they prefer, and leave survivors better able to cope with grief.
Overwhelmingly, patients and families say they want to know prognoses, even if they simultaneously mistrust them.
“There was definite skepticism about whether doctors could predict, but they also thought it would be helpful,” said Dr. White, who has studied surrogates’ wishes. “It helps them prepare for the possibility that their family member may not survive.”
Laura Perry, for instance, is watching her mother grow increasingly debilitated from Parkinson’s disease. Ms. Perry worries about how long her mother, 76, can remain in her assisted living apartment in Glastonbury, Conn. Yet she has found it difficult to persuade her mother’s neurologist to talk about prognosis — even when she sent the physician a note before an appointment, requesting a discussion.
“Doctors always want to be the cheerleaders,” she said.
Not until Ernest Kohn left the I.C.U. for a suburban rehab facility did his family have a frank talk, a conference with his entire medical team, about his future. The doctors explained that Dr. Kohn was extremely ill and his prognosis poor; his intermittent good days (he’d regained some speech) wouldn’t change the likely outcome.

It wasn’t what his son — who emerged “sadder, but calmer” — wanted to hear, but he appreciated their empathetic candor. Having that conversation earlier, he added, “would have set the expectation that rather than the miracle cure we were hoping for, we were moving toward the end of life.” His father died 75 days after his stroke.

http://www.nytimes.com/2016/07/05/health/what-doctors-know-about-how-bad-it-is-and-wont-say.html?_r=0

No comments:

Post a Comment